After 8 years, Richardson Support House is closing. We apologise for any inconvenience.
However, low cost and bulk-billed psychology sessions will still be available through Richardson Community Psychology www.richardson-community-psychology.com/
Warm regards, Karen
Sunday, November 29, 2015
Monday, February 16, 2015
DEPRESSION, ANXIETY AND PROBLEM SOLVING
Both
depression and anxiety result in an urge to “problem solve”. This is a positive adaptive response – after
all it is often problems that result in depression and / or anxiety, so coming
up with solutions will help recovery.1 This sometimes results in “excessive
rumination”, “circular” or “broken record” thinking, where thoughts about the
problem (or problems) just go on and on.
Some
researchers have found that problem solving activities of any kind can reduce
depression. First it satisfies that urge
to problem solve, mastery of the game or puzzle increases confidence and
feelings of control (which are reduced by depression and anxiety), and it is a
good distraction from the negative thinking.
So puzzles and problem solving games can be a helpful strategy for
managing depression.
But
it can become a problem in itself if you get caught up in doing puzzles or playing
games – sometimes depressed people talk about playing endless games of
solitaire or something similar, and even staying up late to keep playing (“because
I’m not sleeping anyway”…). This is not
helpful because it stops you from doing activities that would reduce
depression, encourages procrastination when your motivation is already low, and
reduces time for sleep. When this
happens it’s important to take some steps to stop or reduce the activity.
·
Set some limits on the time allowed to play (for example,
turn off the computer at 9pm)
·
Allow youself some game playing time as a reward for some
helpful activity (physical and social activity are helpful for reducing
stress. For example, I’ll play games for
an hour but first I have to take the dog for a walk…)
·
Make a list of the problems that are keep you awake at
night. Set aside a time to reflect on how
you might practically address some (or all!) of these problems and write them
down. If you can’t think of a solution
or two for any problem, put “research” as an answer. Then later on you could brainstorm with a
friend, look up “Google” for some suggestions, or see a counsellor to talk it
through. When you are going to bed, put
your list on your bedside table. Then
everytime you start to think about the problems, say to yourself ‘it’s ok I
have my list ready and I’ll work on these tomorrow. I can’t do anything about these problems at
2am (3am, 4am whatever)’. This is one of
a type of strategy called “putting away” – there are other strategies for this
which a cousellor could discuss with you.
The last step is to start thinking about a ‘non-threatening’ problem
(not as likely to keep you awake) such as ‘if I could travel anywhere in the
world, where would I like to go?’ or ‘if I had a special dinner next week, who
would I invite or what could I make?’ or something like that. If it has an element of problem solving, the
urge to problem solve is satisfied J
Karen
1. Paul W. Andrews
and J. Anderson Thomson, Jr. summary in Psychological Review that argues for
depression as a pro-survival adaptation that allows for a kind of intense,
isolated problem-solving introspection. Analysis
requires a lot of uninterrupted thought, and depression coordinates many
changes in the body to help people analyze their problems without getting
distracted. In a region of the brain known as the ventrolateral prefrontal
cortex (VLPFC), neurons must fire continuously for people to avoid being
distracted. Studies of depression in
rats show that the 5HT1A receptor is involved in supplying neurons with the
fuel they need to fire, as well as preventing them from breaking down. These
important processes allow depressive rumination to continue uninterrupted with
minimal neuronal damage. (The bright
side of being blue: Depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems,
article in Psychological Review. Jul 2009; 116(3): pages 620–654. )
Subscribe to:
Posts (Atom)